The Evolution of Psychology: History, Approaches, and Questions
Learning Objectives
- Explain how psychology changed from a philosophical to a scientific discipline.
- List some of the most important questions that concern psychologists.
- Outline the basic schools of psychology and how each school has contributed to psychology.
In this
section we will review the history of psychology with a focus on the
important questions that psychologists ask and the major approaches (or
schools) of psychological inquiry. The schools of psychology that we
will review are summarized in Table 1.2 "The Most Important Approaches (Schools) of Psychology", and Figure 1.5 "Timeline Showing Some of the Most Important Psychologists"
presents a timeline of some of the most important psychologists,
beginning with the early Greek philosophers and extending to the present
day. Table 1.2 "The Most Important Approaches (Schools) of Psychology" and Figure 1.5 "Timeline Showing Some of the Most Important Psychologists"
both represent a selection of the most important schools and people; to
mention all the approaches and all the psychologists who have
contributed to the field is not possible in one chapter.
The
approaches that psychologists have used to assess the issues that
interest them have changed dramatically over the history of psychology.
Perhaps most importantly, the field has moved steadily from speculation
about behavior toward a more objective and scientific approach as the
technology available to study human behavior has improved (Benjamin
& Baker, 2004). [1]
There has also been an increasing influx of women into the field.
Although most early psychologists were men, now most psychologists,
including the presidents of the most important psychological
organizations, are women.
Table 1.2 The Most Important Approaches (Schools) of Psychology
School of psychology | Description | Important contributors |
---|---|---|
Structuralism | Uses the method of introspection to identify the basic elements or “structures” of psychological experience | Wilhelm Wundt, Edward B. Titchener |
Functionalism | Attempts to understand why animals and humans have developed the particular psychological aspects that they currently possess | William James |
Psychodynamic | Focuses on the role of our unconscious thoughts, feelings, and memories and our early childhood experiences in determining behavior | Sigmund Freud, Carl Jung, Alfred Adler, Erik Erickson |
Behaviorism | Based on the premise that it is not possible to objectively study the mind, and therefore that psychologists should limit their attention to the study of behavior itself | John B. Watson, B. F. Skinner |
Cognitive | The study of mental processes, including perception, thinking, memory, and judgments | Hermann Ebbinghaus, Sir Frederic Bartlett, Jean Piaget |
Social-cultural | The study of how the social situations and the cultures in which people find themselves influence thinking and behavior | Fritz Heider, Leon Festinger, Stanley Schachter |
Female Psychologists
Although most of the earliest psychologists were
men, women are increasingly contributing to psychology. The first
female president of the American Psychological Association was Mary
Whiton Calkins (1861–1930; lower right). Calkins made significant
contributions to the study of memory and the self-concept. Mahzarin
Banaji (upper left), Marilynn Brewer (upper right), and Linda Bartoshuk
(lower left) all have been recent presidents of the American
Psychological Society.
Sources: Bartoshuk photo courtesy of Linda Bartoshuk, http://www.psychologicalscience.org/?s=bartoshuk. Banaji photo courtesy of Mahzarin Rustum Banaji, http://banaji.socialpsychology.org. Brewer photo courtesy of Marilynn Brewer, http://brewer.socialpsychology.org. Calkins photo courtesy of Vlad Sfichi, http://www.flickr.com/photos/24110800@N08/2779490726.
Timeline Showing Some of the Most Important Psychologists
Although it cannot capture every important
psychologist, this timeline shows some of the most important
contributors to the history of psychology.
Although
psychology has changed dramatically over its history, the most important
questions that psychologists address have remained constant. Some of
these questions follow, and we will discuss them both in this chapter
and in the chapters to come:
- Nature versus nurture. Are genes or environment most influential in determining the behavior of individuals and in accounting for differences among people? Most scientists now agree that both genes and environment play crucial roles in most human behaviors, and yet we still have much to learn about how nature (our biological makeup) and nurture (the experiences that we have during our lives) work together (Harris, 1998; Pinker, 2002). The proportion of the observed differences on characteristics among people (e.g., in terms of their height, intelligence, or optimism) that is due to genetics is known as the heritability of the characteristic, and we will make much use of this term in the chapters to come. We will see, for example, that the heritability of intelligence is very high (about .85 out of 1.0) and that the heritability of extraversion is about .50. But we will also see that nature and nurture interact in complex ways, making the question of “Is it nature or is it nurture?” very difficult to answer.
- Free will versus determinism. This question concerns the extent to which people have control over their own actions. Are we the products of our environment, guided by forces out of our control, or are we able to choose the behaviors we engage in? Most of us like to believe in free will, that we are able to do what we want—for instance, that we could get up right now and go fishing. And our legal system is premised on the concept of free will; we punish criminals because we believe that they have choice over their behaviors and freely choose to disobey the law. But as we will discuss later in the research focus in this section, recent research has suggested that we may have less control over our own behavior than we think we do (Wegner, 2002).
- Accuracy versus inaccuracy. To what extent are humans good information processors? Although it appears that people are “good enough” to make sense of the world around them and to make decent decisions (Fiske, 2003), they are far from perfect. Human judgment is sometimes compromised by inaccuracies in our thinking styles and by our motivations and emotions. For instance, our judgment may be affected by our desires to gain material wealth and to see ourselves positively and by emotional responses to the events that happen to us.
President Barack Obama and Vice President Joe
Biden (left photo) meet with BP executives to discuss the disastrous oil
spill in the Gulf of Mexico (right photo). Psychologists study the
causes of poor judgments such as those made by these executives.
Sources: Obama-Hayward photo courtesy of Pete Souza, http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Obama-hayward.jpg. Pelican photo courtesy of the International Bird Rescue Research Center, http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Pelican_wash_from_oil_spill_Louisiana_13_Dawn_ IBRRC_2010.05.04_B6X2141.jpg.
- Conscious versus unconscious processing. To what extent are we conscious of our own actions and the causes of them, and to what extent are our behaviors caused by influences that we are not aware of? Many of the major theories of psychology, ranging from the Freudian psychodynamic theories to contemporary work in cognitive psychology, argue that much of our behavior is determined by variables that we are not aware of.
- Differences versus similarities. To what extent are we all similar, and to what extent are we different? For instance, are there basic psychological and personality differences between men and women, or are men and women by and large similar? And what about people from different ethnicities and cultures? Are people around the world generally the same, or are they influenced by their backgrounds and environments in different ways? Personality, social, and cross-cultural psychologists attempt to answer these classic questions.
Early Psychologists
The
earliest psychologists that we know about are the Greek philosophers
Plato (428–347 BC) and Aristotle (384–322 BC). These philosophers asked
many of the same questions that today’s psychologists ask; for instance,
they questioned the distinction between nature and nurture and the
existence of free will. In terms of the former, Plato argued on the
nature side, believing that certain kinds of knowledge are innate or
inborn, whereas Aristotle was more on the nurture side, believing that
each child is born as an “empty slate” (in Latin a tabula rasa) and that knowledge is primarily acquired through learning and experience.
The earliest psychologists were the Greek
philosophers Plato (left) and Aristotle. Plato believed that much
knowledge was innate, whereas Aristotle thought that each child was born
as an “empty slate” and that knowledge was primarily acquired through
learning and experience.
Sources: Plato photo courtesy of http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Platon2.jpg. Aristotle photo courtesy of Giovanni Dall'Orto, http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Busto_di_Aristotele_conservato_a_Palazzo_Altaemps,
_Roma._Foto_di_Giovanni_Dall%27Orto.jpg.
European
philosophers continued to ask these fundamental questions during the
Renaissance. For instance, the French philosopher René Descartes
(1596–1650) also considered the issue of free will, arguing in its favor
and believing that the mind controls the body through the pineal gland
in the brain (an idea that made some sense at the time but was later
proved incorrect). Descartes also believed in the existence of innate
natural abilities. A scientist as well as a philosopher, Descartes
dissected animals and was among the first to understand that the nerves
controlled the muscles. He also addressed the relationship between mind
(the mental aspects of life) and body (the physical aspects of life).
Descartes believed in the principle of dualism:
that the mind is fundamentally different from the mechanical body.
Other European philosophers, including Thomas Hobbes (1588–1679), John
Locke (1632–1704), and Jean-Jacques Rousseau (1712–1778), also weighed
in on these issues.
The
fundamental problem that these philosophers faced was that they had few
methods for settling their claims. Most philosophers didn’t conduct any
research on these questions, in part because they didn’t yet know how
to do it, and in part because they weren’t sure it was even possible to
objectively study human experience. But dramatic changes came during the
1800s with the help of the first two research psychologists: the German
psychologist Wilhelm Wundt (1832–1920), who developed a psychology
laboratory in Leipzig, Germany, and the American psychologist William
James (1842–1910), who founded a psychology laboratory at Harvard
University.
Structuralism: Introspection and the Awareness of Subjective Experience
Wundt’s
research in his laboratory in Liepzig focused on the nature of
consciousness itself. Wundt and his students believed that it was
possible to analyze the basic elements of the mind and to classify our
conscious experiences scientifically. Wundt began the field known as structuralism, a school of psychology whose goal was to identify the basic elements or “structures” of psychological experience.
Its goal was to create a “periodic table” of the “elements of
sensations,” similar to the periodic table of elements that had recently
been created in chemistry.
Structuralists used the method of introspection to attempt to create a map of the elements of consciousness. Introspection involves asking research participants to describe exactly what they experience as they work on mental tasks,
such as viewing colors, reading a page in a book, or performing a math
problem. A participant who is reading a book might report, for instance,
that he saw some black and colored straight and curved marks on a white
background. In other studies the structuralists used newly invented
reaction time instruments to systematically assess not only what the
participants were thinking but how long it took them to do so. Wundt
discovered that it took people longer to report what sound they had just
heard than to simply respond that they had heard the sound. These
studies marked the first time researchers realized that there is a
difference between the sensation of a stimulus and the perception
of that stimulus, and the idea of using reaction times to study mental
events has now become a mainstay of cognitive psychology.
Wilhelm Wundt (seated at left) and Edward
Titchener (right) helped create the structuralist school of psychology.
Their goal was to classify the elements of sensation through
introspection.
Sources: Wundt photo courtesy of Kenosis, http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Wundt-research-group.jpg. Titchener photo courtesy of Amaro Studios, http://www.flickr.com/photos/39584782@N08/4197763373.
Perhaps
the best known of the structuralists was Edward Bradford Titchener
(1867–1927). Titchener was a student of Wundt who came to the United
States in the late 1800s and founded a laboratory at Cornell University.
In his research using introspection, Titchener and his students claimed
to have identified more than 40,000 sensations, including those
relating to vision, hearing, and taste.
An
important aspect of the structuralist approach was that it was rigorous
and scientific. The research marked the beginning of psychology as a
science, because it demonstrated that mental events could be quantified.
But the structuralists also discovered the limitations of
introspection. Even highly trained research participants were often
unable to report on their subjective experiences. When the participants
were asked to do simple math problems, they could easily do them, but
they could not easily answer how they did
them. Thus the structuralists were the first to realize the importance
of unconscious processes—that many important aspects of human psychology
occur outside our conscious awareness, and that psychologists cannot
expect research participants to be able to accurately report on all of
their experiences.
Functionalism and Evolutionary Psychology
In
contrast to Wundt, who attempted to understand the nature of
consciousness, the goal of William James and the other members of the
school of functionalism was to understand why animals and humans have developed the particular psychological aspects that they currently possess (Hunt, 1993).
For James, one’s thinking was relevant only to one’s behavior. As he
put it in his psychology textbook, “My thinking is first and last and
always for the sake of my doing” (James, 1890).
James and the other members of the functionalist school were influenced by Charles Darwin’s (1809–1882) theory of natural selection,
which proposed that the physical characteristics of animals and humans
evolved because they were useful, or functional. The functionalists
believed that Darwin’s theory applied to psychological characteristics
too. Just as some animals have developed strong muscles to allow them to
run fast, the human brain, so functionalists thought, must have adapted
to serve a particular function in human experience.
The functionalist school of psychology,
founded by the American psychologist William James (left), was
influenced by the work of Charles Darwin.
Source: James photo courtesy of http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:William_James,_philosopher.jpg. Darwin portrait courtesy of George Richmond, http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Charles_Darwin_by_G._Richmond.jpg.
Although
functionalism no longer exists as a school of psychology, its basic
principles have been absorbed into psychology and continue to influence
it in many ways. The work of the functionalists has developed into the
field of evolutionary psychology, a branch of psychology that applies the Darwinian theory of natural selection to human and animal behavior (Dennett, 1995; Tooby & Cosmides, 1992).
Evolutionary psychology accepts the functionalists’ basic assumption,
namely that many human psychological systems, including memory, emotion,
and personality, serve key adaptive functions. As we will see in the
chapters to come, evolutionary psychologists use evolutionary theory to
understand many different behaviors including romantic attraction,
stereotypes and prejudice, and even the causes of many psychological
disorders.
A key component of the ideas of evolutionary psychology is fitness. Fitness refers to the
extent to which having a given characteristic helps the individual
organism survive and reproduce at a higher rate than do other members of
the species who do not have the characteristic. Fitter organisms
pass on their genes more successfully to later generations, making the
characteristics that produce fitness more likely to become part of the
organism’s nature than characteristics that do not produce fitness. For
example, it has been argued that the emotion of jealousy has survived
over time in men because men who experience jealousy are more fit than
men who do not. According to this idea, the experience of jealously
leads men to be more likely to protect their mates and guard against
rivals, which increases their reproductive success (Buss, 2000).
Despite
its importance in psychological theorizing, evolutionary psychology
also has some limitations. One problem is that many of its predictions
are extremely difficult to test. Unlike the fossils that are used to
learn about the physical evolution of species, we cannot know which
psychological characteristics our ancestors possessed or did not
possess; we can only make guesses about this. Because it is difficult to
directly test evolutionary theories, it is always possible that the
explanations we apply are made up after the fact to account for observed
data (Gould & Lewontin, 1979).
Nevertheless, the evolutionary approach is important to psychology
because it provides logical explanations for why we have many
psychological characteristics.
Psychodynamic Psychology
Perhaps the school of psychology that is most familiar to the general public is the psychodynamic approach to understanding behavior, which was championed by Sigmund Freud (1856–1939) and his followers. Psychodynamic psychology is an approach to understanding human behavior that focuses on the role of unconscious thoughts, feelings, and memories.
Freud developed his theories about behavior through extensive analysis
of the patients that he treated in his private clinical practice. Freud
believed that many of the problems that his patients experienced,
including anxiety, depression, and sexual dysfunction, were the result
of the effects of painful childhood experiences that the person could no
longer remember.
Sigmund Freud and the other psychodynamic
psychologists believed that many of our thoughts and emotions are
unconscious. Psychotherapy was designed to help patients recover and
confront their “lost” memories.
Source: Photo courtesy of Max Halberstadt, http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Sigmund_Freud_LIFE.jpg.
Freud’s
ideas were extended by other psychologists whom he influenced,
including Carl Jung (1875–1961), Alfred Adler (1870–1937), Karen Horney
(1855–1952), and Erik Erikson (1902–1994). These and others who follow
the psychodynamic approach believe that it is possible to help the
patient if the unconscious drives can be remembered, particularly
through a deep and thorough exploration of the person’s early sexual
experiences and current sexual desires. These explorations are revealed
through talk therapy and dream analysis, in a process called psychoanalysis.
The
founders of the school of psychodynamics were primarily practitioners
who worked with individuals to help them understand and confront their
psychological symptoms. Although they did not conduct much research on
their ideas, and although later, more sophisticated tests of their
theories have not always supported their proposals, psychodynamics has
nevertheless had substantial impact on the field of psychology, and
indeed on thinking about human behavior more generally (Moore &
Fine, 1995).
The importance of the unconscious in human behavior, the idea that
early childhood experiences are critical, and the concept of therapy as a
way of improving human lives are all ideas that are derived from the
psychodynamic approach and that remain central to psychology.
Behaviorism and the Question of Free Will
Although
they differed in approach, both structuralism and functionalism were
essentially studies of the mind. The psychologists associated with the
school of behaviorism, on the other hand, were
reacting in part to the difficulties psychologists encountered when
they tried to use introspection to understand behavior. Behaviorism is a
school of psychology that is based on the premise that it is not
possible to objectively study the mind, and therefore that psychologists
should limit their attention to the study of behavior itself.
Behaviorists believe that the human mind is a “black box” into which
stimuli are sent and from which responses are received. They argue that
there is no point in trying to determine what happens in the box because
we can successfully predict behavior without knowing what happens
inside the mind. Furthermore, behaviorists believe that it is possible
to develop laws of learning that can explain all behaviors.
The
first behaviorist was the American psychologist John B. Watson
(1878–1958). Watson was influenced in large part by the work of the
Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov (1849–1936), who had discovered that
dogs would salivate at the sound of a tone that had previously been
associated with the presentation of food. Watson and the other
behaviorists began to use these ideas to explain how events that people
and other organisms experienced in their environment (stimuli) could produce specific behaviors (responses). For instance, in Pavlov’s research the stimulus (either the food or, after learning, the tone) would produce the response of salivation in the dogs.
In
his research Watson found that systematically exposing a child to
fearful stimuli in the presence of objects that did not themselves
elicit fear could lead the child to respond with a fearful behavior to
the presence of the stimulus (Watson & Rayner, 1920; Beck, Levinson,
& Irons, 2009).
In the best known of his studies, an 8-month-old boy named Little
Albert was used as the subject. Here is a summary of the findings:
The boy was placed in the middle of a room; a white laboratory rat was placed near him and he was allowed to play with it. The child showed no fear of the rat. In later trials, the researchers made a loud sound behind Albert’s back by striking a steel bar with a hammer whenever the baby touched the rat. The child cried when he heard the noise. After several such pairings of the two stimuli, the child was again shown the rat. Now, however, he cried and tried to move away from the rat.
In line with the behaviorist approach, the boy had learned to associate the white rat with the loud noise, resulting in crying.
B. F. Skinner was a member of the
behaviorist school of psychology. He argued that free will is an
illusion and that all behavior is determined by environmental factors.
Source: Photo courtesy of http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:B.F._Skinner_at_Harvard_circa _1950.jpg.
The
most famous behaviorist was Burrhus Frederick (B. F.) Skinner
(1904–1990), who expanded the principles of behaviorism and also brought
them to the attention of the public at large. Skinner used the ideas of
stimulus and response, along with the application of rewards or reinforcements,
to train pigeons and other animals. And he used the general principles
of behaviorism to develop theories about how best to teach children and
how to create societies that were peaceful and productive. Skinner even
developed a method for studying thoughts and feelings using the
behaviorist approach (Skinner, 1957, 1968, 1972).
Research Focus: Do We Have Free Will?
The
behaviorist research program had important implications for the
fundamental questions about nature and nurture and about free will. In
terms of the nature-nurture debate, the behaviorists agreed with the
nurture approach, believing that we are shaped exclusively by our
environments. They also argued that there is no free will, but rather
that our behaviors are determined by the events that we have experienced
in our past. In short, this approach argues that organisms, including
humans, are a lot like puppets in a show who don’t realize that other
people are controlling them. Furthermore, although we do not cause our
own actions, we nevertheless believe that we do because we don’t realize
all the influences acting on our behavior.
Recent
research in psychology has suggested that Skinner and the behaviorists
might well have been right, at least in the sense that we overestimate
our own free will in responding to the events around us (Libet, 1985;
Matsuhashi & Hallett, 2008; Wegner, 2002). In one demonstration of the misperception of our own free will, neuroscientists Soon, Brass, Heinze, and Haynes (2008) placed their research participants in a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)
brain scanner while they presented them with a series of letters on a
computer screen. The letter on the screen changed every one-half second.
The participants were asked, whenever they decided to, to press either
of two buttons. Then they were asked to indicate which letter was
showing on the screen when they decided to press the button. The
researchers analyzed the brain images to see if they could predict which
of the two buttons the participant was going to press, even before the
letter at which he or she had indicated the decision to press a button.
Suggesting that the intention to act occurred in the brain before the
research participants became aware of it, the researchers found that the
prefrontal cortex region of the brain showed activation that could be
used to predict the button press as long as 10 seconds before the
participants said that they decided which button to press.
Research has
found that we are more likely to think that we control our behavior when
the desire to act occurs immediately prior to the outcome, when the
thought is consistent with the outcome, and when there are no other
apparent causes for the behavior. Aarts, Custers, and Wegner (2005)
asked their research participants to control a rapidly moving square
along with a computer that was also controlling the square
independently. The participants pressed a button to stop the movement.
When participants were exposed to words related to the location of the
square just before they stopped its movement, they became more likely to
think that they controlled the motion, even when it was actually the
computer that stopped it. And Dijksterhuis, Preston, Wegner, and Aarts
(2008)
found that participants who had just been exposed to first-person
singular pronouns, such as “I” and “me,” were more likely to believe
that they controlled their actions than were people who had seen the
words “computer” or “God.”
The idea that
we are more likely to take ownership for our actions in some cases than
in others is also seen in our attributions for success and failure.
Because we normally expect that our behaviors will be met with success,
when we are successful we easily believe that the success is the result
of our own free will. When an action is met with failure, on the other
hand, we are less likely to perceive this outcome as the result of our
free will, and we are more likely to blame the outcome on luck or our
teacher (Wegner, 2003).
The behaviorists made substantial contributions to psychology by identifying the principles of learning.
Although the behaviorists were incorrect in their beliefs that it was
not possible to measure thoughts and feelings, their ideas provided new
ideas that helped further our understanding regarding the nature-nurture
debate as well as the question of free will. The ideas of behaviorism
are fundamental to psychology and have been developed to help us better
understand the role of prior experiences in a variety of areas of
psychology.
The Cognitive Approach and Cognitive Neuroscience
Science
is always influenced by the technology that surrounds it, and
psychology is no exception. Thus it is no surprise that beginning in the
1960s, growing numbers of psychologists began to think about the brain
and about human behavior in terms of the computer, which was being
developed and becoming publicly available at that time. The analogy
between the brain and the computer, although by no means perfect,
provided part of the impetus for a new school of psychology called cognitive psychology. Cognitive psychology is a field of psychology that studies mental processes, including perception, thinking, memory, and judgment. These actions correspond well to the processes that computers perform.
Although
cognitive psychology began in earnest in the 1960s, earlier
psychologists had also taken a cognitive orientation. Some of the
important contributors to cognitive psychology include the German
psychologist Hermann Ebbinghaus (1850–1909), who studied the ability of
people to remember lists of words under different conditions, and the
English psychologist Sir Frederic Bartlett (1886–1969), who studied the
cognitive and social processes of remembering. Bartlett created short
stories that were in some ways logical but also contained some very
unusual and unexpected events. Bartlett discovered that people found it
very difficult to recall the stories exactly, even after being allowed
to study them repeatedly, and he hypothesized that the stories were
difficult to remember because they did not fit the participants’
expectations about how stories should go. The idea that our memory is
influenced by what we already know was also a major idea behind the
cognitive-developmental stage model of Swiss psychologist Jean Piaget
(1896–1980). Other important cognitive psychologists include Donald E.
Broadbent (1926–1993), Daniel Kahneman (1934–), George Miller (1920–),
Eleanor Rosch (1938–), and Amos Tversky (1937–1996).
The War of the Ghosts
The War of the Ghosts
was a story used by Sir Frederic Bartlett to test the influence of
prior expectations on memory. Bartlett found that even when his British
research participants were allowed to read the story many times they
still could not remember it well, and he believed this was because it
did not fit with their prior knowledge.
One night two young men from Egulac went down to the river to hunt seals and while they were there it became foggy and calm. Then they heard war-cries, and they thought: “Maybe this is a war-party.” They escaped to the shore, and hid behind a log. Now canoes came up, and they heard the noise of paddles, and saw one canoe coming up to them. There were five men in the canoe, and they said:“What do you think? We wish to take you along. We are going up the river to make war on the people.”One of the young men said, “I have no arrows.”“Arrows are in the canoe,” they said.“I will not go along. I might be killed. My relatives do not know where I have gone. But you,” he said, turning to the other, “may go with them.”So one of the young men went, but the other returned home.And the warriors went on up the river to a town on the other side of Kalama. The people came down to the water and they began to fight, and many were killed. But presently the young man heard one of the warriors say, “Quick, let us go home: that Indian has been hit.” Now he thought: “Oh, they are ghosts.” He did not feel sick, but they said he had been shot.So the canoes went back to Egulac and the young man went ashore to his house and made a fire. And he told everybody and said: “Behold I accompanied the ghosts, and we went to fight. Many of our fellows were killed, and many of those who attacked us were killed. They said I was hit, and I did not feel sick.”He told it all, and then he became quiet. When the sun rose he fell down. Something black came out of his mouth. His face became contorted. The people jumped up and cried.He was dead. (Bartlett, 1932)
Cognitive psychologists, such as (from left
to right) Jean Piaget, George Miller, and Eleanor Rosch work to
understand how people learn, remember, and make judgments about the
world around them.
Sources: Piaget photo courtesy of David Kauppi, http://www.flickr.com/photos/vansterpsykologerna/3407151541/in/photostream. Miller photo courtesy of Association for Psychological Science http://www.psychologicalscience.org/anniversary/timeline.cfm. Rosch photo courtesy of the University of Pittsburgh, http://www.sis.pitt.edu/~mbsclass/hall_of_fame/rosch.html.
In
its argument that our thinking has a powerful influence on behavior, the
cognitive approach provided a distinct alternative to behaviorism.
According to cognitive psychologists, ignoring the mind itself will
never be sufficient because people interpret the stimuli that they
experience. For instance, when a boy turns to a girl on a date and says,
“You are so beautiful,” a behaviorist would probably see that as a
reinforcing (positive) stimulus. And yet the girl might not be so easily
fooled. She might try to understand why the boy is making this
particular statement at this particular time and wonder if he might be
attempting to influence her through the comment. Cognitive psychologists
maintain that when we take into consideration how stimuli are evaluated
and interpreted, we understand behavior more deeply.
Cognitive
psychology remains enormously influential today, and it has guided
research in such varied fields as language, problem solving, memory,
intelligence, education, human development, social psychology, and
psychotherapy. The cognitive revolution has been given even more life
over the past decade as the result of recent advances in our ability to
see the brain in action using neuroimaging techniques. Neuroimaging is the use of various techniques to provide pictures of the structure and function of the living brain (Ilardi & Feldman, 2001).
These images are used to diagnose brain disease and injury, but they
also allow researchers to view information processing as it occurs in
the brain, because the processing causes the involved area of the brain
to increase metabolism and show up on the scan. We have already
discussed the use of one neuroimaging technique, functional magnetic
resonance imaging (fMRI), in the research focus earlier in this section,
and we will discuss the use of neuroimaging techniques in many areas of
psychology in the chapters to follow.
Social-Cultural Psychology
A
final school, which takes a higher level of analysis and which has had
substantial impact on psychology, can be broadly referred to as the social-cultural approach. The field of social-cultural psychology is the study of how the social situations and the cultures in which people find themselves influence thinking and behavior.
Social-cultural psychologists are particularly concerned with how
people perceive themselves and others, and how people influence each
other’s behavior. For instance, social psychologists have found that we
are attracted to others who are similar to us in terms of attitudes and
interests (Byrne, 1969), that we develop our own beliefs and attitudes by comparing our opinions to those of others (Festinger, 1954), and that we frequently change our beliefs and behaviors to be similar to those of the people we care about—a process known as conformity.
An important aspect of social-cultural psychology are social norms—the ways of thinking, feeling, or behaving that are shared by group members and perceived by them as appropriate (Asch, 1952; Cialdini, 1993).
Norms include customs, traditions, standards, and rules, as well as the
general values of the group. Many of the most important social norms
are determined by the culture in which we live, and these cultures are studied by cross-cultural psychologists. A culture represents the
common set of social norms, including religious and family values and
other moral beliefs, shared by the people who live in a geographical
region (Fiske, Kitayama, Markus, & Nisbett, 1998; Markus, Kitayama, & Heiman, 1996; Matsumoto, 2001).
Cultures influence every aspect of our lives, and it is not
inappropriate to say that our culture defines our lives just as much as
does our evolutionary experience (Mesoudi, 2009).
Psychologists
have found that there is a fundamental difference in social norms
between Western cultures (including those in the United States, Canada,
Western Europe, Australia, and New Zealand) and East Asian cultures
(including those in China, Japan, Taiwan, Korea, India, and Southeast
Asia). Norms in Western cultures are primarily oriented toward individualism,
which is about valuing the self and one’s independence from others.
Children in Western cultures are taught to develop and to value a sense
of their personal self, and to see themselves in large part as separate
from the other people around them. Children in Western cultures feel
special about themselves; they enjoy getting gold stars on their
projects and the best grade in the class. Adults in Western cultures are
oriented toward promoting their own individual success, frequently in
comparison to (or even at the expense of) others.
Norms in the East Asian culture, on the other hand, are oriented toward interdependence or collectivism.
In these cultures children are taught to focus on developing harmonious
social relationships with others. The predominant norms relate to group
togetherness and connectedness, and duty and responsibility to one’s
family and other groups. When asked to describe themselves, the members
of East Asian cultures are more likely than those from Western cultures
to indicate that they are particularly concerned about the interests of
others, including their close friends and their colleagues.
In Western cultures social norms promote a focus on the self (individualism), whereas in Eastern cultures the focus is more on families and social groups (collectivism).
Another
important cultural difference is the extent to which people in
different cultures are bound by social norms and customs, rather than
being free to express their own individuality without considering social
norms (Chan, Gelfand, Triandis, & Tzeng, 1996).
Cultures also differ in terms of personal space, such as how closely
individuals stand to each other when talking, as well as the
communication styles they employ.
It
is important to be aware of cultures and cultural differences because
people with different cultural backgrounds increasingly come into
contact with each other as a result of increased travel and immigration
and the development of the Internet and other forms of communication. In
the United States, for instance, there are many different ethnic
groups, and the proportion of the population that comes from minority
(non-White) groups is increasing from year to year. The social-cultural
approach to understanding behavior reminds us again of the difficulty of
making broad generalizations about human nature. Different people
experience things differently, and they experience them differently in
different cultures.
The Many Disciplines of Psychology
Psychology
is not one discipline but rather a collection of many subdisciplines
that all share at least some common approaches and that work together
and exchange knowledge to form a coherent discipline (Yang & Chiu,
2009).
Because the field of psychology is so broad, students may wonder which
areas are most suitable for their interests and which types of careers
might be available to them. Table "Some Career Paths in Psychology"
will help you consider the answers to these questions. You can learn
more about these different fields of psychology and the careers
associated with them at http://www.apa.org/careers/psyccareers/.
Table -Some Career Paths in Psychology
Psychology field | Description | Career opportunities |
---|---|---|
Biopsychology and neuroscience | This field examines the physiological bases of behavior in animals and humans by studying the functioning of different brain areas and the effects of hormones and neurotransmitters on behavior. | Most biopsychologists work in research settings—for instance, at universities, for the federal government, and in private research labs. |
Clinical and counseling psychology | These are the largest fields of psychology. The focus is on the assessment, diagnosis, causes, and treatment of mental disorders. | Clinical and counseling psychologists provide therapy to patients with the goal of improving their life experiences. They work in hospitals, schools, social agencies, and in private practice. Because the demand for this career is high, entry to academic programs is highly competitive. |
Cognitive psychology | This field uses sophisticated research methods, including reaction time and brain imaging to study memory, language, and thinking of humans. | Cognitive psychologists work primarily in research settings, although some (such as those who specialize in human-computer interactions) consult for businesses. |
Developmental psychology | These psychologists conduct research on the cognitive, emotional, and social changes that occur across the lifespan. | Many work in research settings, although others work in schools and community agencies to help improve and evaluate the effectiveness of intervention programs such as Head Start. |
Forensic psychology | Forensic psychologists apply psychological principles to understand the behavior of judges, attorneys, courtroom juries, and others in the criminal justice system. | Forensic psychologists work in the criminal justice system. They may testify in court and may provide information about the reliability of eyewitness testimony and jury selection. |
Health psychology | Health psychologists are concerned with understanding how biology, behavior, and the social situation influence health and illness. | Health psychologists work with medical professionals in clinical settings to promote better health, conduct research, and teach at universities. |
Industrial-organizational and environmental psychology | Industrial-organizational psychology applies psychology to the workplace with the goal of improving the performance and well-being of employees. | There are a wide variety of career opportunities in these fields, generally working in businesses. These psychologists help select employees, evaluate employee performance, and examine the effects of different working conditions on behavior. They may also work to design equipment and environments that improve employee performance and reduce accidents. |
Personality psychology | These psychologists study people and the differences among them. The goal is to develop theories that explain the psychological processes of individuals, and to focus on individual differences. | Most work in academic settings, but the skills of personality psychologists are also in demand in business—for instance, in advertising and marketing. PhD programs in personality psychology are often connected with programs in social psychology. |
School and educational psychology | This field studies how people learn in school, the effectiveness of school programs, and the psychology of teaching. | School psychologists work in elementary and secondary schools or school district offices with students, teachers, parents, and administrators. They may assess children’s psychological and learning problems and develop programs to minimize the impact of these problems. |
Social and cross-cultural psychology | This field examines people’s interactions with other people. Topics of study include conformity, group behavior, leadership, attitudes, and person perception. | Many social psychologists work in marketing, advertising, organizational, systems design, and other applied psychology fields. |
Sports psychology | This field studies the psychological aspects of sports behavior. The goal is to understand the psychological factors that influence performance in sports, including the role of exercise and team interactions. | Sports psychologists work in gyms, schools, professional sports teams, and other areas where sports are practiced. |
Psychology in Everyday Life: How to Effectively Learn and Remember
One way that
the findings of psychological research may be particularly helpful to
you is in terms of improving your learning and study skills.
Psychological research has provided a substantial amount of knowledge
about the principles of learning and memory. This information can help
you do better in this and other courses, and can also help you better
learn new concepts and techniques in other areas of your life.
The most
important thing you can learn in college is how to better study, learn,
and remember. These skills will help you throughout your life, as you
learn new jobs and take on other responsibilities. There are substantial
individual differences in learning and memory, such that some people
learn faster than others. But even if it takes you longer to learn than
you think it should, the extra time you put into studying is well worth
the effort. And you can learn to learn—learning to effectively study and
to remember information is just like learning any other skill, such as
playing a sport or a video game.
To learn well,
you need to be ready to learn. You cannot learn well when you are
tired, when you are under stress, or if you are abusing alcohol or
drugs. Try to keep a consistent routine of sleeping and eating. Eat
moderately and nutritiously, and avoid drugs that can impair memory,
particularly alcohol. There is no evidence that stimulants such as
caffeine, amphetamines, or any of the many “memory enhancing drugs” on
the market will help you learn (Gold, Cahill, & Wenk, 2002;
McDaniel, Maier, & Einstein, 2002).
Memory supplements are usually no more effective than drinking a can of
sugared soda, which also releases glucose and thus improves memory
slightly.
Psychologists
have studied the ways that best allow people to acquire new information,
to retain it over time, and to retrieve information that has been
stored in our memories. One important finding is that learning is an
active process. To acquire information most effectively, we must
actively manipulate it. One active approach is rehearsal—repeating the
information that is to be learned over and over again. Although simple
repetition does help us learn, psychological research has found that we
acquire information most effectively when we actively think about or
elaborate on its meaning and relate the material to something else.
When you
study, try to elaborate by connecting the information to other things
that you already know. If you want to remember the different schools of
psychology, for instance, try to think about how each of the approaches
is different from the others. As you make the comparisons among the
approaches, determine what is most important about each one and then
relate it to the features of the other approaches. In an important study
showing the effectiveness of elaborative encoding, Rogers, Kuiper, and
Kirker (1977) found that students learned information best when they related it to aspects of themselves (a phenomenon known as the self-reference effect). This research suggests that imagining how the material relates to your own interests and goals will help you learn it.
An approach known as the method of loci
involves linking each of the pieces of information that you need to
remember to places that you are familiar with. You might think about the
house that you grew up in and the rooms in it. Then you could put the
behaviorists in the bedroom, the structuralists in the living room, and
the functionalists in the kitchen. Then when you need to remember the
information, you retrieve the mental image of your house and should be
able to “see” each of the people in each of the areas.
One of the most fundamental principles of learning is known as the spacing effect.
Both humans and animals more easily remember or learn material when
they study the material in several shorter study periods over a longer
period of time, rather than studying it just once for a long period of
time. Cramming for an exam is a particularly ineffective way to learn.
Psychologists
have also found that performance is improved when people set difficult
yet realistic goals for themselves (Locke & Latham, 2006).
You can use this knowledge to help you learn. Set realistic goals for
the time you are going to spend studying and what you are going to
learn, and try to stick to those goals. Do a small amount every day, and
by the end of the week you will have accomplished a lot.
Our ability to adequately assess our own knowledge is known as metacognition.
Research suggests that our metacognition may make us overconfident,
leading us to believe that we have learned material even when we have
not. To counteract this problem, don’t just go over your notes again and
again. Instead, make a list of questions and then see if you can answer
them. Study the information again and then test yourself again after a
few minutes. If you made any mistakes, study again. Then wait for a half
hour and test yourself again. Then test again after 1 day and after 2
days. Testing yourself by attempting to retrieve information in an
active manner is better than simply studying the material because it
will help you determine if you really know it.
In summary,
everyone can learn to learn better. Learning is an important skill, and
following the previously mentioned guidelines will likely help you learn
better.
Key Takeaways
- The first psychologists were philosophers, but the field became more empirical and objective as more sophisticated scientific approaches were developed and employed.
- Some basic questions asked by psychologists include those about nature versus nurture, free will versus determinism, accuracy versus inaccuracy, and conscious versus unconscious processing.
- The structuralists attempted to analyze the nature of consciousness using introspection.
- The functionalists based their ideas on the work of Darwin, and their approaches led to the field of evolutionary psychology.
- The behaviorists explained behavior in terms of stimulus, response, and reinforcement, while denying the presence of free will.
- Cognitive psychologists study how people perceive, process, and remember information.
- Psychodynamic psychology focuses on unconscious drives and the potential to improve lives through psychoanalysis and psychotherapy.
- The social-cultural approach focuses on the social situation, including how cultures and social norms influence our behavior.
No comments:
Post a Comment